Remember me

I Knew It!

February 23, 2010

            OK, I’ll get back to the Greatest Pitcher’s Duels tomorrow or the next day, but I wanted to take off today to study a couple of other things that I can study with this wonderful data base that Isaac created for me, which has all the game lines for pitchers in the Retrosheet files (Thank you, Retrosheet.)

            It is universally known and often observed that modern pitchers do not stay in the game as long as they once did.   It was once common for pitchers to pitch complete games—and quite common for pitchers to stay in the game, if necessary, beyond the 9th inning.  This is no longer common.

            However, there is another side to this issue which is, as far as I know, totally unexamined and undocumented.    It has long seemed to me that, as long outings have disappeared, so have short outings.   I spend a lot of time going through Retrosheet box scores, looking at games from decades past.   With what seems to me stunning regularity, you find these old games in which the starting pitcher gives up two, three runs in the second inning—and comes out of the game.

            The way they thought about it was different from the way we think about it.  The way they thought was, “if this pitcher doesn’t have his good stuff today, we’ve got to get him out and get somebody into the game who does.”   This would happen not only to second-line pitchers, but even to good pitchers—more often to second-line pitchers, of course, but not exclusively.   It would happen more often in a big game.   If it was a big game, in the way that 1950s, 1960s managers thought about it, then we really need to get the pitcher out of there quickly if he doesn’t have his good stuff.

            This was part and parcel of the irregular usage patterns of baseball in years past—pitchers switching between the bullpen and the starting staff, coming into the game off-rotation, starting on long rest or short rest.   Another part of it—I have thought, but never documented and never seen documented—was that pitchers also got jerked out of the game very early in some games.

 

            OK, is that true?

 

            It’s much more true than I would have guessed.   Since the 1950s, the number of “unexplained early departures” has decreased in every decade—and has decreased, start to finish, by almost 80%.

            I defined an “unexplained early departure” as “any game in which the starting pitcher is taken out of the game before completing four innings and before allowing five runs, but not including games in which the starting pitcher is taken out before allowing any runs.”   Earned runs or un-earned; if the pitcher has given up five runs, then we can understand his being taken out of the game.  We didn’t include games in which the pitcher gave up no runs, because those must be injury games or special situations of some kind, rather than the events we are looking for.

            In the 1950s we have data for 18,362 games (9,181 games, but both sides.)    In 2,072 of those games, the starting pitcher came out of the game having allowed less than five runs and having thrown less than four innings.   That’s 11.3%.    In the 1960s, this went down to 9.5%:

 

 

 

Unexplained

 

 

 

Early

 

Decade

Starts

Departures

Percentage

1950s

18362

2072

11.3%

1960s

31730

3006

9.5%

 

            By the 1980s, it had dropped to 5.7%:

 

 

 

Unexplained

 

 

 

Early

 

Decade

Starts

Departures

Percentage

1950s

18362

2072

11.3%

1960s

31730

3006

9.5%

1970s

39560

2764

7.0%

1980s

40674

2302

5.7%

 

            And by the last decade, to 2.4%:

 

 

 

Unexplained

 

 

 

Early

 

Decade

Starts

Departures

Percentage

1950s

18362

2072

11.3%

1960s

31730

3006

9.5%

1970s

39560

2764

7.0%

1980s

40674

2302

5.7%

1990s

43188

1500

3.5%

2000s

48582

1147

2.4%

 

            Not to hype the discovery, but. . .it seems like a pretty important thing to know.   People talk a lot about the pitch limits, about pitchers not staying in to finish the game the way they did years ago, etc.   It seems to me that the real situation is a little different than the way people imagine it to be.    It is not merely that pitchers come out earlier, but also that they stay in the game now, in some situations where, in years past, they would come out.   Not saying that the two sides of the sword are the same or that they balance, but. . .it doesn’t just cut one way.

            I looked at the data in a second way, to make certain that I wasn’t accidentally creating the pattern by the way that I defined an early exit.   I made up an “exit score”, which is this:

 

            One point for each out recorded by the pitcher, plus

            Six points for each run allowed, plus

            the Square of innings pitched. 

 

            So if a pitcher pitches 7 innings and allows 3 runs, that would be an exit score of 88—21 for the 21 outs recorded, plus 18 for the 3 runs allowed, plus 49 because 49 is the square of 7.   If it was 7 1/3, I’d add 53.78, as 7 1/3 squared is 53.78.   That’s about where most pitchers come out of the game—around 85, 90.   Sometimes, of course, a little earlier, sometimes later, but somewhere in that range.

            When a pitcher comes out of the game with an exit score less than 30, that’s a very early exit.    In this study, I counted all games in which the pitcher left with an exit score of less than 30.   This shows the same pattern as before.   The percentage of very early exits has gone down in every decade within the range of the study, and, start to finish, almost 80% of the very early exits have disappeared:

 

 

 

Unexplained

 

 

Exit Score

 

 

 

Early

 

 

Less than

 

Decade

Starts

Departures

Percentage

30

Percentage

1950s

18362

2072

11.3%

 

1113

6.1%

1960s

31730

3006

9.5%

 

1473

4.6%

1970s

39560

2764

7.0%

 

1259

3.2%

1980s

40674

2302

5.7%

 

938

2.3%

1990s

43188

1500

3.5%

 

660

1.5%

2000s

48582

1147

2.4%

 

620

1.3%

 

While I was doing that, I decided that I might as well look as well at the number of “Hard Work Exits”, defining a “Hard Work Exit” as any game with an exit score greater than 125.   This shows what you would expect it to show:

 

 

 

 

Exit Score

 

 

Exit Score

 

 

 

 

Less than

 

 

Over

 

Decade

Starts

 

30

Percentage

125

Percentage

1950s

18362

 

1113

6.1%

2064

11.2%

1960s

31730

 

1473

4.6%

2239

7.1%

1970s

39560

 

1259

3.2%

3003

7.6%

1980s

40674

 

938

2.3%

1669

4.1%

1990s

43188

 

660

1.5%

544

1.3%

2000s

48582

 

620

1.3%

178

0.4%

 

            The number of long, arm-destroying outings has also declined in every decade except the 1970s, and the overall decline is about 97%.

            I also did one more test.   For each decade, I figured the average of the 100 MOST abusive outings by a starting pitcher.  This is not exactly standard, since the number of outings has increased; if we were really pursuing a conclusion here, rather than illustrating what is already known, we would have to use the average of the top 1% or something.   But I just used the average of the top 100, the hundred “hardest” outings for pitchers.  This is the data:

 

1950s

214.98

1960s

206.63

1970s

196.12

1980s

166.97

1990s

139.64

2000s

132.31

 

            But we knew that anyway.   Let me try again to explain the significance of what we did not know.   People think that modern managers are only concerned, and are over-concerned, with not over-working their starting pitchers.   That’s not exactly true.   Rather, in the modern world we believe in regular workloads.   Whereas managers in the fifties and sixties believed that some days pitchers just didn’t “have it”, and jerked the pitcher out of the game quickly when they thought he didn’t have it, we believe now that you get maximum production and minimal risk of injury with a  regular, predictable workload.   And, really, this is progress, because the whole thing about the pitcher just “not having it today” is mostly just nonsense.   If you give up a couple of runs in the second inning, that doesn’t mean you don’t have it today; it just means you gave up a couple of runs in the second inning.   It is a mistake to let that kind of thing pull you off course from what you are trying to accomplish.

 

            OK, my other research issue here is, by contrast, just idle curiosity.   For many years I have wondered what was the most common pitcher’s box score line.    You know those lines that pitchers have in the box score, like this:

 

           

IP

H

R

ER

BB

SO

6

7

3

2

2

2

6

2

2

2

5

6

1 1/3

5

4

4

3

0

9

3

0

0

5

4

8

7

3

3

5

5

10

8

6

4

4

3

5

6

2

2

3

0

5

7

4

4

3

1

0

2

4

4

2

0

9

6

1

1

8

8

4

8

7

7

4

0

5 1/3

6

4

4

4

1

8

6

3

3

8

5

 

            For years I have wondered. . .how many different lines are there?  Does each “line” get used every year?   Do pitchers repeat lines?    Since I have this data, I can now examine those kind of questions. . .not that it matters a whit to anybody, but I was just curious.

            These are the most common box score pitcher’s lines, by decade, within the data that we have:

 

 

Decade

IP

H

R

ER

BB

SO

 

1950s

9

6

1

1

2

5

Occurred 16 times

1960s

9

3

0

0

1

5

Occurred 25 times

1970s

9

4

0

0

2

4

Occurred 22 times

1980s

9

6

0

0

1

4

Occurred 19 times

1990s

7

5

2

2

1

5

Occurred 24 times

2000s

7

5

1

1

1

4

Occurred 30 times

 

 

            There are, it turns out, a very, very large number of pitcher’s box score lines that occur in any season, so that if you look at one season by itself, you will find that only a fairly small percentage of the lines are duplicated at all.   Even the lines that occur most often—those above—account for less that 1/1,000th of all game lines, so that the “most common” lines are not typical lines. 

            Taking the six decades as a whole, these are the 22 most common box score lines, and the number of times that each has occurred, and the won-lost record when it has occurred:

 

   

IP

 

H

R

ER

BB

SO

 

Has Occurred

Wins

Losses

7

 

5

2

2

1

3

 

80

32

25

9

 

6

1

1

1

5

 

78

77

1

7

 

5

2

2

2

4

 

78

29

17

7

 

5

2

2

1

4

 

78

36

13

9

 

5

0

0

1

4

 

77

76

0

9

 

4

0

0

2

5

 

75

73

0

9

 

4

0

0

1

4

 

75

74

0

9

 

6

0

0

1

5

 

74

74

0

9

 

5

1

1

1

4

 

74

69

3

7

 

5

1

1

1

4

 

74

45

5

7

 

6

2

2

1

4

 

73

31

21

7

 

7

2

2

1

3

 

72

36

19

9

 

4

0

0

1

5

 

69

67

0

7

 

5

2

2

2

3

 

69

27

21

9

 

6

1

1

2

4

 

67

62

4

7

 

6

2

2

1

3

 

67

35

13

9

 

5

1

1

1

3

 

66

62

1

9

 

5

0

0

1

6

 

66

65

0

9

 

4

0

0

2

4

 

66

65

0

9

 

5

0

0

2

5

 

65

64

0

7

 

6

1

1

1

5

 

65

46

4

7

 

5

2

2

1

5

 

65

29

18

 

            The most common box score line that usually results in a loss is  6   7  3  3  2  3.  That’s occurred 62 times in our data, with a won-lost record of 18-24.

 

            In the 1950s, with 18,362 game lines in our data, there were 13,581 different pitcher’s lines that occurred.   40% of game lines were lines occurred more than once; 60% occurred only once.

            As you have more games, of course, you have more lines that are “hit” more than once, so the percentage of “unique lines” has gone down every decade since.   Here, let me chart the data:

 

 

Game Lines

Unique Pitchers Lines

Percentage of Games with Multiple-Use Lines

Percentage Used Once

1950s

18362

13851

40.4%

59.6%

1960s

31730

20951

50.0%

50.0%

1970s

39560

25497

51.9%

48.1%

1980s

40674

25692

54.3%

45.7%

1990s

43188

26359

56.3%

43.7%

2000s

48582

26454

62.7%

37.3%

           

The percentage of multiple-use pitcher’s lines has gone up, I think, not only because there are more games, thus more opportunities to step on a line that has already been used, but also because of the phenomenon noted in the first half of this article, which is the disappearance on both ends of the “extreme games”.   Pitchers don’t pitch 11 innings in a game anymore—and they don’t come out after two.   This tends to concentrate more games in the middle.   But still, even in modern baseball lines, 37% of the box scores lines for pitchers are lines that occur only once during the decade.

            In the years 1952-2009 inclusive (as much as we have in the data), there are 81,562 distinct pitcher’s box score lines which occur.   Of these, 50,626 occur only once in the data, and those account for 22.8% of all games.

            If any of this is on Jeopardy! tomorrow, I’ll be expecting a cut.

 
 

COMMENTS (12 Comments, most recent shown first)

fmilder
Taking into account that 62 times is hardly a large number, I still think that it's interesting that a "standard" quality start (6 innings, 9 base runners, 3 ER) results in a significantly losing record (18-24). Should someone do an analysis of what a true quality start is (i.e., something that at least results in a 50-50, maybe better, won-loss record)?
6:57 AM Mar 12th
 
THBR
Long memory, Cooper! (AND either you've been a subscriber for a while, or you like to read [and maybe re-read] everything.)

Funny line!
12:50 PM Mar 3rd
 
Cooper
When i saw the title of the article my first thought was "someone found the Lemur".
9:27 AM Mar 1st
 
barronmo
If I'm understanding this correctly- in the past a pitcher does something in a game to meet "exit criterion", and he exits whether it's the second inning of the seventh. Now the exit criterion only apply later in the game, presumably because pitch count has reached a certain level. If the exit criterion is bunk in the second, why is it valid in the seventh? Could one compare games where the pitcher meets exit criterion but doesn't come out with games where he does? Who comes out ahead? Can one define an exit criterion that produces a big difference between these groups?
11:51 PM Feb 27th
 
birtelcom
I had the same reaction as Phil -- because you are defining "unexplained early departures" with a fixed runs scored threshold, I wonder whether the decline in such events merely reflects the fact that run scoring has increased in the majors each decade from the 60s on. That would suggest that what seems "unexplained" today looks more and more explicable as you go back each decade to the 60s.
10:25 PM Feb 26th
 
110phil
I always thought that there were fewer two- and three-inning starts because of the inflated run environment ... it used to be that if you gave up five runs, you were having a very bad day, but now, it's just par for the course.

Any evidence for that hypothesis?
5:34 PM Feb 24th
 
PeteRidges
Great article.

Between 1954 and 1980, 16 pitchers pitched complete games in which they were charged with at least 9 runs. It hasn't happened since.

Highest exit score since 1954: 168, Mike Norris (Oakland, 1980), 9 IP, 10 runs, L:
http://www.baseball-reference.com/boxes/OAK/OAK198009260.shtml

Highest exit score ever: 760, Leon Cadore (Dodgers, 1920) and Joe Oeschger (Braves, 1920), 26 IP, 1 run, tie:
for further details, google for [cadore oeschger "box score"]


9:59 AM Feb 24th
 
THBR
The MOST common line occurred 30 (that's T-H-I-R-T-Y) times?? I mean, I know every baseball game is not the same as every other baseball game, but (to me, anyway), that is simply incredible. Another reason to read Bill James ....

PLEASE, if you have any other "idle curiosity" items, spit 'em out!

And thank YOU, Isaac, for helping yer old man out! (:8-{D#>
7:38 PM Feb 23rd
 
wovenstrap
I think I know why the percentage of line that are used once has declined, even as the average length of start has become so much more standardized (5-9 innings, say). Taken in isolation, you would have to expect that the frequency of identical lines would be increasing too, but that has not happened. I think the reason is that the 2000s have been a run-happy environment. Think about all those games in Coors, for instance. If you're going 6 innings and giving up 7 runs as a matter of routine (relatively), that would explain a ton of variance in the resultant pitcher's lines. People giving up 7 runs on 7 hits, 7 runs on 15 hits, 8 runs on 6 hits and 5 walks, etc etc.

I think this also addresses OntoWaveland's question. A strong line, if you lock in a variable like exactly 7, 8, or 9 innings pitched -- will strongly tend to have great similarity in the H, R, and ER columns too -- a 2-hit shutout is a common "thing" in the way an 6.2-inning, 6-run effort just isn't. And then you get reasonably common K/BB results, like 8 Ks/2 BBs, and whammo, you've got an easy thing to match.

Also, consider that when you're pitching poorly, you're much more likely to be taken out in the middle of an inning, and those partial innings also probably have a great effect in causing additional variance in the lines. When hitters are hitting, you get more 6.1, 6.2, 7.1, 7.2 inning efforts, and that causes variance. Is that something we can test? Is it more common for starters to be taken out from the mound rather than between innings? I'd bet yes, just because relief appearances are so high, but I don't know.
5:29 PM Feb 23rd
 
Kev
It would be interesting how hitting (by decade) looked when bullpen performance only were studied. From the "clean-ball' inception "1920's" to the time when specialization first took hold. I have often thought that one reason hitters reached some of the high totals thry did was because they feasted off bullpen pitching. The perception of washed up, no-place-else-to-put-them members, if true, would account for some of the monster numbers of the 30's and 40's HOFers as well as next level elite hitters. It's not like Jimmy Foxx had to face Goose Gossage when the starter faltered.
12:27 PM Feb 23rd
 
Trailbzr
Did this happen because the role of starter and reliever have become more specialized?
A starter is someone who's supposed to use four pitches for six innings; a reliever pitches one inning using two. A modern bullpen isn't really designed to have a 4+ inning guy.
12:12 PM Feb 23rd
 
dburba
Any reason why the most common pitching lines are without exception "good" results, and in a lot of cases excellent? Very interesting stuff.
12:02 PM Feb 23rd
 
 
©2024 Be Jolly, Inc. All Rights Reserved.|Powered by Sports Info Solutions|Terms & Conditions|Privacy Policy